Time Travel
Transcript
Today, we visit yesterday and
tomorrow. The University of Houston's College of Engineering presents this
series about the machines that make our civilization run, and the people whose
ingenuity created them.
I'm ten years old, questioning my
father about what's possible and what's not. "How about flying faster than
sound," I ask. "Oh, I think that'll happen someday," he replies.
Indeed, it did -- just eight years later. "How about a rocket ship to the
moon," I wonder. "I think so," he answers, "but not in our
lifetimes." Yet we got there 29 years later, with both of us still quite
alive.
But this is 1940, and I press on:
"Fly faster than light?" He looks at the ceiling, speculatively,
"I don't know; I don't think so." Time for the 64-dollar question:
"How about a time machine; will we ever travel in time machines?"
This time he shakes his head and says, "No, I believe that really is
impossible."
My father, a newspaper writer, knew
that change was afoot. In 1925, long before anyone had split the atom, he'd
done a newspaper editorial on the future of power from nuclear fission. But
time travel lay outside even his speculations. And, for me, it remains right up
there with tooth fairies, world peace and safe cigarettes.
Still, I'm a sucker for a good
time-travel yarn. Those stories first took the form of H. G. Wells' The Time
Machine. Vehicles we could ride back and forth along time's single track.
Later, people became increasingly
aware of paradoxes that might occur if we went back and messed with the past.
Twelve years after the conversation with my father, Ray Bradbury played that
theme with chilling force in his short story, A Sound of Thunder. In it,
tourists visit the past. One accidentally crushes a butterfly -- then returns
to a horribly altered present.
My wife and I read and enjoyed a new
variation in the book, The Time Traveler’s Wife. Forget science fiction. Now
time travel is a literary device. The woman's life follows linear time. The
man's life pops up, out of sequence, in random segments of hers. The story is
more about information flow than time travel. Tracing who knows what, and when,
makes a tantalizing jigsaw puzzle.
Our latest foray, the movie The Lake
House, plays another leitmotif on time travel. What travels now is pure
information. The instrument isn't a machine, but a magical mailbox through
which letters pass across a two year gap. Critics who view the movie as science
fiction hate it. Those who see it as a parable about opportunities missed and
claimed, like it a lot.
Maybe that's why I like time travel
stories. They remind me that I live either in a deterministic universe, with
only one time line, or I can choose opportunities. If I could go back in time
with freedom of choice, you and I would never know. That self's choices would
return him to a different present -- not so our present.
As writers of time travel stories
recognize that fact, they contrive new and more interesting stories, ones that
recognize how choice creates a lack of symmetry between past and present. After
all, future travel -- that's something we all do, all the time.
I'm John Lienhard, at the University
of Houston, where we're interested in the way inventive minds work.
Time Idioms & Clichés
IDIOMS
beat the clock = do something within
the deadline.
pressed for time = not have much
time.
make good time = do something faster
than you thought.
in the nick of time = at the last
possible instant.
high time = the right time.
hardly the time = an inappropriate
time for something.
lose track of time = forget about
the time.
do time = serve a prison sentence.
ahead of one’s time = be
forward-thinking.
time and a half = get paid for
overtime.
time warp = stuck in a past time
a.made good time b. stuck in a time warp c.pressed for time d. doing time e.in the nick of time f. high time
g.beat the clock h.lost all track
of time. i. ahead of his time. j.hardly
the time
Think You're Multitasking? Think Again
1.b 2.a
3.a 4.a 5.b
6.a
Transcript
Think You're Multitasking?
Think Again
Don't believe the multitasking hype, scientists say. New research shows
that we humans aren't as good as we think we are at doing several things at
once. But it also highlights a human skill that gave us an evolutionary edge.
As technology allows people to do more tasks at the same time, the myth
that we can multitask has never been stronger. But researchers say it's still a
myth — and they have the data to prove it.
Humans, they say, don't do lots of things simultaneously. Instead, we
switch our attention from task to task extremely quickly.
A case example, researchers say, is a group of people who focus not on a
BlackBerry but on a blueberry — as in pancakes.
To make it as a short-order cook, you must be able to keep a half-dozen
orders in your head while cracking eggs, flipping pancakes, working the
counter, and refilling coffee cups.
And at a restaurant like the Tastee Diner, in Bethesda, Md., the orders
come in verbally, not on a ticket.
Chocolate chip pancakes, scrambled with sausage, order of french fries,
rye toast — they're small tasks. On a busy day, though, they add up to a tough
job for Shawn Swinson.
"My first month here, I was ready to walk out the door," he
said.
Asked what it feels like when he's in the middle of rush hour, Swinson
said, "Like you're in an insane asylum. It's almost unbearable."
Swinson has learned to handle the pressure. He's an island of calm, even
when the orders are flying. But Swinson's boss, manager Frank Long, says very
few people can keep up without losing their cool.
"It's singularly the most difficult job in this type of
operation," Long said. "Four cooks. Five waitresses. Bus staff. Host.
Getting them in and out."
Speed and accuracy are at a premium — especially when the customers are
multitasking, too. Lunchtime is the worst, Long said.
"People may have an errand to run. Maybe go to the bank and pick up
dry cleaning, and eat. All within an hour, whatever time they have."
It's all part of life these days. We answer e-mails while yapping on the
phone. We schedule appointments while driving and listening to the radio. And
it seems as if we're focusing on all these tasks simultaneously, as if we've
become true masters of doing 10 things at once.
But, brain researchers say, that's not really the case.
Multitasking: A Human Delusion?
"People can't multitask very well, and when people say they can,
they're deluding themselves," said neuroscientist Earl Miller. And, he
said, "The brain is very good at deluding itself."
Miller, a Picower professor of neuroscience at MIT, says that for the
most part, we simply can't focus on more than one thing at a time.
What we can do, he said, is shift our focus from one thing to the next
with astonishing speed.
"Switching from task to task, you think you're actually paying
attention to everything around you at the same time. But you're actually
not," Miller said.
"You're not paying attention to one or two things simultaneously,
but switching between them very rapidly."
Miller said there are several reasons the brain has to switch among tasks.
One is that similar tasks compete to use the same part of the brain.
"Think about writing an e-mail and talking on the phone at the same
time. Those things are nearly impossible to do at the same time," he said.
"You cannot focus on one while doing the other. That's because of
what's called interference between the two tasks," Miller said. "They
both involve communicating via speech or the written word, and so there's a lot
of conflict between the two of them."
Researchers say they can actually see the brain struggling. And now
they're trying to figure out the details of what's going on.
Putting The Mind To The Test
At a lab at the University of Michigan, researchers are using an MRI
scanner to photograph test subjects' brains as they take on different tasks.
During a recent test, Daniel Weissman, the neuroscientist in charge of
the experiment, explained that a man lying inside the scanner would be
performing different tasks, depending on the color of two numbers he sees on a
screen.
"If the two digits are one color — say, red — the subject decides
which digit is numerically larger," Weissman said. "On the other
hand, if the digits are a different color — say green — then the subject
decides which digit is actually printed in a larger font size."
The tests can be tricky — which is the point. After an attempt, the
technician told the test subject, "OK, do the same thing, except try to go
faster this time."
MRI studies like this one, Weissman said, have shown that when the man
in the scanner sees green, his brain has to pause before responding — to round
up all the information it has about the green task.
When the man sees red, his brain pauses again — to push aside
information about the green task and replace it with information about the red
task.
If the tasks were simpler, they might not require this sort of
full-throttle switching. But, Weissman said, even simple tasks can overwhelm
the brain when we try to do several at once.
"If I'm out on a street corner and I'm looking for one friend who's
wearing a red scarf, I might be able to pick out that friend," Weissman
said.
"But if I'm looking for a friend who's wearing a red scarf on one
street corner, and in the middle of the street I'm looking for another friend
who's wearing a blue scarf — and on the other side of the street I'm looking
for a friend wearing a green scarf — at some point, I can only divide my
attention so much, and I begin to have trouble."
So the brain starts switching. Scan for red. Switch. Scan for blue.
Switch. Scan for green. Switch.
The part of the brain that does this is called the "executive
system." It's a bit like one of those cartoon conductors telling the
orchestra: louder, softer, faster, slower. You come in here. You be quiet for a
few measures.
The conductor in our heads lives in the brain's frontal lobes, basically
above our eyes.
"Executive processes allow us to make plans for our future
behaviors," Weissman said. "They allow us to exert some sort of
voluntary control over our behavior."
The executive system also helps us achieve a goal by ignoring
distractions.
"For example, if we're performing a task where we want to watch TV
and ignore voices that are coming from, say, our children nearby,"
Weissman said, "our frontal region brain may configure the brain to
prioritize visual information and dampen down auditory information."
And the brain's executive will keep us in that mode until we hear, say,
one of our children screaming.
"These are the things that make us the most human," Weissman
said. "We are not like jellyfish — it's not like when you poke us, we
always do the same thing."
A Role In Evolution
Humans are also not like cats, or dogs, or even apes, when it comes to
controlling how our brain responds, and what it responds to. Weissman says this
skill probably evolved to help humans — who are pretty vulnerable, physically —
to do things like hunt animals that are bigger and stronger.
"As hunters, you know, people had to hunt something, and keep track
of where their friends were," Weissman said. "You've got to think
about, 'What is that tiger going to do?' you know, and, 'I've got my group of
friends' — and surround the tiger."
Weissman says that keeping track of all those things wouldn't be
possible without the executive system in our frontal lobes.
Still, Weissman said, "There are lots of animals in the world that
hunt without these increased abilities. So I wouldn't say that to hunt you have
to have a lot of frontal development.
"But on the other hand, it helps. That's why humans have become
dominant on the planet."
Dominant — and, perhaps, too confident in our own skill. Studies show
that we frequently overestimate our ability to handle multiple tasks.
For early humans, that sort of miscalculation could have meant becoming
a tiger's lunch. These days, the consequences are more likely to be stress, a
blunder — or maybe a car crash.
No comments:
Post a Comment